Humans are storytelling beings. As far as we know, no other species has the capacity for language and ability to use it in endlessly creative ways. From our earliest days, we name and describe things. We tell others what’s happening around us.
For people immersed in the study of language and the study of learning, one really important question has engendered a lot of debate over the years: How much of this ability is innate — part of our genetic makeup — and how much do we learn from our environments?
There’s no doubt that we acquire our native languages, complete with their vocabularies and grammatical patterns.
But is there an inherited ability underlying our individual languages — a structural framework that enables us to grasp, retain, and develop language so easily?
In 1957, linguist Noam Chomsky published a groundbreaking book called “Syntactic Structures.” It proposed a novel idea: All human beings may be born with an innate understanding of how language works.
Whether we learn Arabic, English, Chinese, or sign language is determined, of course, by the circumstances of our lives.
But according to Chomsky, we can acquire language because we’re genetically encoded with a universal grammar — a basic understanding of how communication is structured.
Chomsky’s idea has since become widely accepted.
Languages share certain basic traits
Chomsky and other linguists have said that all languages contain similar elements. For example, globally speaking, language breaks down into similar categories of words: nouns, verbs, and adjectives, to name three.
Another shared characteristic of language is
For example, take the structure of a descriptor. In almost every known language, it’s possible to repeat descriptors over and over again: “She wore an itsy-bitsy, teeny-weeny, yellow polka dot bikini.”
Strictly speaking, more adjectives could be added to further describe that bikini, each embedded within the existing structure.
The recursive property of language allows us to expand the sentence “She believed Ricky was innocent” almost endlessly: “Lucy believed that Fred and Ethel knew Ricky had insisted he was innocent.”
The recursive property of language is sometimes called “nesting,” because in almost all languages, sentences can be expanded by placing repeating structures inside each other.
Chomsky and others have argued that because almost all languages share these characteristics despite their other variations, we may be born preprogrammed with a universal grammar.
We learn language almost effortlessly
Linguists like Chomsky have argued for a universal grammar in part because children everywhere develop language in very similar ways in short periods of time with little assistance.
Children show awareness of language categories at extremely early ages, long before any overt instruction occurs.
For example, one study showed that 18-month-old children recognized “a doke” referred to a thing and “praching” referred to an action, showing they understood the form of the word.
Having the article “a” before it or ending with “-ing” determined whether the word was an object or an event.
It’s possible they had learned these ideas from listening to people talk, but those who espouse the idea of a universal grammar say it’s more likely that they have an innate understanding of how words function, even if they don’t know the words themselves.
And we learn in the same sequence
Proponents of universal grammar say children the world over naturally develop language in the same sequence of steps.
So, what does that shared developmental pattern look like? Many linguists agree that there are three basic stages:
- learning sounds
- learning words
- learning sentences
More specifically:
- We perceive and produce speech sounds.
- We babble, usually with a consonant-then-vowel pattern.
- We speak our first rudimentary words.
- We grow our vocabularies, learning to classify things.
- We build two-word sentences, and then increase the complexity of our sentences.
Different children proceed through these stages at different rates. But the fact that we all share the same developmental sequence may show we’re hardwired for language.
We learn despite a ‘poverty of stimulus’
Chomsky and others have also argued that we learn complex languages, with their intricate grammatical rules and limitations, without receiving explicit instruction.
For example, children automatically grasp the correct way to arrange dependent sentence structures without being taught.
We know to say “The boy who is swimming wants to eat lunch” instead of “The boy wants to eat lunch who is swimming.”
Despite this lack of instructional stimulus, we still learn and use our native languages, understanding the rules that govern them. We wind up knowing a lot more about how our languages work than we’re ever overtly taught.
Noam Chomsky is among the most oft-quoted linguists in history. Nevertheless, there’s been lots of debate around his universal grammar theory for over half a century now.
One fundamental argument is that he’s got it wrong about a biological framework for language acquisition. Linguists and educators who differ with him say we acquire language the same way we learn everything else: through our exposure to stimuli in our environment.
Our parents speak to us, whether verbally or using signs. We “absorb” language by listening to conversations taking place all around us, from the subtle corrections we receive for our linguistic errors.
For instance, a child says, “I no want that.”
Their caregiver responds, “You mean, ‘I don’t want that.’”
But Chomsky’s theory of universal grammar doesn’t deal with how we learn our native languages. It’s focused on the innate capacity that makes all our language learning possible.
A more fundamental
Take recursion, for example. There are languages that simply aren’t recursive.
And if the principles and parameters of language aren’t really universal, how could there be an underlying “grammar” programmed into our brains?
One of the most practical outgrowths has been the idea that there’s an optimal age for language acquisition among children.
The younger, the better is the prevailing idea. Since young children are primed for natural language acquisition, learning a second language may be more effective in early childhood.
The universal grammar theory has also had a profound influence on classrooms where students are learning second languages.
Many teachers now use more natural, immersive approaches that mimic the way we acquire our first languages, rather than memorizing grammatical rules and vocabulary lists.
Teachers who understand universal grammar may also be better prepared to explicitly focus on the structural differences between students’ first and second languages.
Noam Chomsky’s theory of universal grammar says that we’re all born with an innate understanding of the way language works.
Chomsky based his theory on the idea that all languages contain similar structures and rules (a universal grammar), and the fact that children everywhere acquire language the same way, and without much effort, seems to indicate that we’re born wired with the basics already present in our brains.
Although not everyone agrees with Chomsky’s theory, it continues to have a profound influence on how we think about language acquisition today.